Human neuropsychology II: Consequences of callosal dysfunctioning
Callosal size in children with learning disabilities

https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90133-3Get rights and content

Abstract

The corpus callosum (CC), the main structure subserving hemispheric collaboration, that is necessary for efficient cognitive functioning, undergoes developmental processes such as axonal retraction and myelination. Callosal growth therefore is vulnerable for adverse events such as perinatal asphyxia, but there are also genetic and epigenetic factors that determine form and thickness. MRI scans of 110 children, either with specific learning disabilities (LD), i.e. dysphasia/dyslexia, or with several degrees of general LD, showed callosa that were highly variable in size. The callosal size corrected for brain size did not vary significantly according to the severity of the LD, although it tended to be smaller in severe LD, i.e. mental retardation. Callosal size varied however, due to the likely presence of genetic influences or of adverse perinatal events. Children with familial dysphasia/dyslexia, had a thicker CC, possibly reflecting a poorly understood neurodevelopmental mechanism that inhibits the establishment of cerebral dominance. LD children (all subgroups together) with perinatal adverse events had a smaller CC than the familial cases, suggesting CC damage. Despite a multitude of developmental factors influencing the final size, this study suggests that total callosal size, supposedly linked to interhemispheric function, may contribute to the pathophysiological mechanisms that give rise to LD.

Reference (38)

  • DietrichR.B. et al.

    MR evaluation of early myelination patterns in normal and developmentally delayed infants

    Am. J. Radiol.

    (1988)
  • DubowitzL.M.S. et al.

    Developmental sequence of periventricular leucomalacia

    Arch. Dis. Childhood

    (1985)
  • FarberD. et al.

    Electrophysiological correlates of interhemispheric interaction in ontogenesis

  • GladstoneN. et al.

    Anomalous bimanual coordination among dyslexic boys

    Dev. Psychol.

    (1989)
  • HayakawaK. et al.

    Development and aging of brain mindline structures: assessment with MR imaging

    Neuroradiology

    (1989)
  • HyndG.W. et al.

    Corpus callosum morphology in attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: morphometric analysis of MRI

    J. Learn. Dis.

    (1991)
  • InnocentiG.M.

    General organization of callosal connections in the cerebral cortex

  • JanowskyJ.S. et al.

    The outcome of perinatal brain damage: the role of normal neuron loss and axon retraction

    Dev. Med. Child Neurol.

    (1986)
  • LangmeierM. et al.

    Effect of prolonged hypobaric hypoxia during postnatal development on myelination of the corpus callosum in rats

    J. Hirnforsch.

    (1987)
  • Cited by (68)

    • Axon diameter inferences in the human corpus callosum using oscillating gradient spin echo sequences

      2022, Magnetic Resonance Imaging
      Citation Excerpt :

      Axon diameter and nerve conduction velocity are linearly related, and the diameter is crucial to determine because of its association with many medical conditions in vivo [2,9–12]. Previous studies have indicated that the size and distribution of axons may be altered in diseases including Alzheimer's [13], amyotrophic lateral sclerosis [14–16], autism [17,18], diabetes [19], dyslexia [20], multiple sclerosis [21], and schizophrenia [22,23]. The first MRI temporal diffusion spectroscopy methods applied to infer axon diameter sizes [2,6,24,25] used single diffusion encoding sequences [1,26] such as the pulsed gradient spin echo (PGSE) pulse sequence [27].

    • White matter microstructure from nonparametric axon diameter distribution mapping

      2016, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Conventional neuroanatomical methods applied to postmortem human or animal tissue have provided evidence that the ADD changes in neurological conditions such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) (Cluskey and Ramsden, 2001) and multiple sclerosis (MS) (Trapp et al., 1998; Evangelou et al., 2001; Lovas et al., 2000). In addition, several pathologies, including autism (Hughes, 2007), dyslexia (Njiokiktjien et al., 1994), schizophrenia (Randall, 1983), and even alcoholism (Livy and Elberger, 2008), have been associated with changes in the distribution of axon size. Changes in the number of axons and their diameters also take place throughout normal development accompanying the period of dynamic behavioral, cognitive, and emotional changes in childhood and adolescence (Yakovlev, 1967; Pfefferbaum et al., 1994; Gregg et al., 2007; Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Schlaug et al., 2009).

    • Including diffusion time dependence in the extra-axonal space improves in vivo estimates of axonal diameter and density in human white matter

      2016, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, changes in axonal morphology have been associated with many conditions of interest. For instance, alterations of axonal diameters have been observed in psychiatric conditions like autism (Piven et al., 1997; Hughes, 2007), dyslexia (Njiokiktjien et al., 1994) and schizophrenia (Randall, 1983; Rice and Barone, 2000). Axon diameters were found to change following exposure to alcohol (Livy and Elberger, 2008), and increase in diameter of the axonal initial segment was reported as an early change in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Sasaki and Maruyama, 1992).

    • Spatiotemporal and frequency signatures of word recognition in the developing brain: A magnetoencephalographic study

      2013, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Two hypotheses may explain some of the neural underpinnings of our observations: (1) The myelination and white-matter development that occurs between ages 6 to 17 may underlie such decreases in latency (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Halgren et al., 2002; Klingberg et al., 1999; Pujol et al., 2006; Schmithorst et al., 2008; Su et al., 2008). Furthermore, it has been seen that problems in developmental myelination cause learning deficits and language impairment (Njiokiktjien et al., 1994). If our observations were only white-matter related, we expect that latencies in many of the peaks would decrease with brain maturation.

    • Brain Abnormalities in Language Disorders and in Autism

      2007, Pediatric Clinics of North America
      Citation Excerpt :

      Yet another study found volume reduction in the anterior of the corpus [70]. In SLI, one study found no difference in midsagittal callosal area [62], whereas another measured the corpus to be thicker in children who had familial dysphasia/dyslexia [71]. In our own samples [66], we found no difference in either SLI or autism in the midsagittal area of the corpus callosum, either as a whole or in any of the specific subregions delineated according to the method of Witelson [72]; however, the lack of volume increase occurred in the setting of larger brain and white matter volume, which ought to have lead to a larger corpus callosum because corpus callosum normally covaries to the 2/3 power of brain volume [73].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text